To Learn English, Bilingual Children Need Robust Vocabulary
Children who hear a rich vocabulary acquire a rich vocabulary and children who hear a rich vocabulary in full sentences acquire the ability to put all of their words together in full sentences.
The way a highly proficient speaker of English talks to a child is different from the way a less proficient speaker does. Even for two-year-olds, these differences matter in how they acquire and develop language skills. Early language exposure provides the foundation for children鈥檚 language development. Therefore, the factors that shape that language exposure, shape language development.
A new study by researchers at 澳门六合彩历史记录 and collaborators at digs deep into these differences and finds that differences in the vocabulary used while talking to children affect how useful that speech is to the child trying to figure out the patterns of language. The bottom line? The quality of child-directed speech depends on the language proficiency of the speaker. Children who hear a rich vocabulary acquire a rich vocabulary and children who hear a rich vocabulary in full sentences acquire the ability to put all of their words together in full sentences.
Findings from this study, published in the , have broad implications for immigrant parents鈥 language choices at home and for staffing practices in early care and education centers.聽
Many children learn language, in part, from the speech of non-native speakers who vary in their language proficiency. Even though there is evidence for the effects of language input from native and non-native parents on bilingual children鈥檚 speech such as acoustic properties, no research to date has investigated the effects of vocabulary and grammar on a child鈥檚 acquisition of English. Vocabulary size is the most reliably observed difference between native speakers and even quite proficient non-native speakers.聽
鈥淛ust because a parent speaks English better than their two-year-old, doesn鈥檛 mean that they speak English well enough to maximally support the child鈥檚 acquisition of English,鈥 said , Ph.D., lead author and a professor of in 澳门六合彩历史记录鈥檚 . 鈥淐hildren can鈥檛 learn what they don鈥檛 hear. So if their parents or other adults speak to them with a very simple, limited vocabulary that is all that they can learn.鈥
For the study,Hoff and collaborators compared three groups of mothers: mothers who were native speakers of English; mothers who were not native speakers of English but rated themselves as highly proficient; and mothers who were not native speakers of English and rated themselves as having limited proficiency. All of these mothers talked to their children in English on a regular basis.
Results of the study provide evidence suggesting that the child-directed speech of native speakers and non-native speakers with good proficiency provide a richer database for language acquisition than the child-directed speech of speakers with limited proficiency. Native speakers consistently showed the highest level of the supportive properties of input in their child-directed speech, followed by the non-native speakers with good proficiency, and the non-native speakers with limited proficiency who consistently showed the lowest levels.
The speakers with limited proficiency illustrated a smaller vocabulary for their children to learn, used those words in shorter utterances, and their utterances contained less diversity in the lexical items that fill grammatical roles, compared to native English speakers.
The study also found being educated from an English-speaking college or university also matters even if the non-native speaker has advanced degrees from another country. Hoff suggests that one way to become proficient in a language and proficient in a way that translates into a benefit for the child is to be educated in that language.
鈥淓ven if you are highly educated from another country and you are not proficient in English, when you speak to your child in English it will show. There really is something about being proficient in English that helps you talk to your child in a way that benefits his or her language,鈥 said Hoff. 鈥淧arents who are more proficient in English use a richer vocabulary and produce longer sentences, even when they are talking to a toddler. Clearly, all of the mothers know as much as their two-year-olds, but the difference between what you know when you can barely speak a language and what you know when you鈥檙e highly proficient in a language translates into a difference that matters in children, and that鈥檚 really the key point of this study.鈥
Because differences also are observed among native speakers of English in their talk to adults and children, the authors suggest providing more educational opportunities for native speakers as a route to closing other language gaps, such as those associated with socioeconomic status.
To observe speech between mother and child for the study, researchers videotaped them playing together. They transcribed everything that the mothers said to their two-and-a-half year-old children using a special software program as well as manual transcription to count the number of different words and utterances, and to measure the length of sentences.聽
Co-authors of the study are Katherine F. Shanks in 澳门六合彩历史记录鈥檚 Department of Psychology, and , Ph.D., an associate professor in the Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences at The George Washington University.
This research was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
-澳门六合彩历史记录-
Tags: science | faculty and staff | research | broward